top of page
White Structure

Academic confidence and dyslexia at university

Conclusions and reflections

By yellowcloud from Germany - Laserbeam Glass Plate Fluorescence, CC BY 2.0, https://commons.wikimedia.org/w/index.php?curid=8541495

6.1  Summarizing the purpose of the research

 

... and reflecting on the wider implications

This study focused on trying to understand more about how the academic confidence of university students with dyslexia may be affected by their dyslexia. The research arose from a desire to apply scientific process to anecdotally observed evidence about how dyslexic students tackle their studies in comparison to their non-dyslexic peers. At two different university settings in my professional positions as an academic guide, my experience of working with both groups of students to develop their learning (and meta-learning), indicated that considerable differences exist in attitudes and behaviours in relation to academic study.

 

It is acknowledged that these can arise through a variety of individual circumstances and learning situations, both current and historical. However, the learning difference of dyslexia uniquely sets apart a substantial minority of students from their mainstream peers as a consequence of the ways that their dyslexia impacts on their academic studies, not least in comparison to learning impacts attributed to other minority-group characteristics, such as ethnicity, social class or cultural differences. This is because dyslexia presents unique challenges in literacy-based education systems, challenges which are based on the assumption that dyslexia is fundamentally an issue associated with literacy capabilities. The evidence for this is substantial, and not a point of specific argument in this thesis. However, there is also considerable evidence that in high-functioning adult learners, as typically seen at university, many of the earlier literacy challenges inherent to a dyslexic individual’s learning processes may have been strategically ameliorated, leaving other dimensions of dyslexia to emerge, potentially to have a significant impact on actions and behaviours in academic study.

 

Many learners face issues that appear to be directly related to their approaches to their academic challenges (Klassen, 2006). Examples have been cited in the literature review above. However Klassen’s view especially resonates with the themes in this project by arguing that poor confidence can be the source of many learning challenges because academic confidence is the bridge that connects an individual's self-efficacy beliefs to their absolute performance in an academic task. This is important because it implies that academic confidence is a constituent, success-forecast component of the process that students progress through from facing a specific academic task demand to the academic output that is their submission.

 

This process will be partly a function of metacognitive knowledge and partly a function of intrinsic capabilities as the most likely drivers of sound, academic progress. In Section 2 it was shown that significant earlier studies have explored these ideas from the standpoint of students with dyslexia: For example, it has been shown that dyslexic students struggle with analysing task requirements, and they often focus on lower-skill competencies such as spelling and grammar, while not recognizing the need for organizational capabilities or writing in a particular register (Butler, 1998, 1999); and that dyslexic students can be less metacognitively aware than their non-dyslexic peers but that this may be more of a manifestation of dyslexic students' knowledge (or self-perception) that both their own, but perhaps more significantly, external expectations of the quality of their academic output is reduced (Tunmer and Chapman, 1996). These negative perceptions of academic competency may be driven by stigma associated with the disability label (Ho, 2004), but also by reduced levels of confidence (in comparison to their peers) about how successful their approaches to meeting the persistent challenges of studying at university will be.

 

Some of the outcomes of this current study suggest that these characteristics may not be unique to students with dyslexia. Evidence has been presented to demonstrate that students more widely across the university community find organizing, managing and judging the complexity of their academic workload to be challenging. However, for students with dyslexia these factors may be more acute. Studies have shown that depressed academic expectations, for example, may be a consequence of experiences in earlier learning, such that these learners had built perceptions that less was being demanded of them academically, or even that educational opportunities were being denied to them because of their dyslexia (Shifrer, 2013; Shifrer et al., 2013; Hornstra et al., 2014). In this current study, the ABC Scales used to gauge academic confidence do not explore the relationship between that metric and academic expectations. But where the full, 24-dimension scale has been shown to be just as effective in a reduced, 17-item format, it is suggested that this spare capacity may permit a scale-item revision that could subsequently include dimensions designed to gauge students' expectations. This might be a useful development of the scale which would be no more burdensome to complete than the original, 24-item version, but which may then provide a more comprehensive profile of academic confidence.

 

But it is also possible that dyslexic students' disability status may have resulted in their prior learning experiences being littered with teachers who misjudged their academic potential by being more focused on managing their apparent disability (Hurwitz et al., 2007). Evidence from the qualitative data collected in not only this current study, but also from the Master's project conducted nearly a decade earlier, suggests that this may persist into higher education where study skills support, well-intentioned as it undoubtedly is, adds to study pressure and anxiety rather than acts to ameliorate it for many students with dyslexia:

"... I am unable to use study support sessions as I am already finding it hard to keep

up with course work and don't have time." (Respondent #34, Dykes, 2008, p89)

"Going for help with studies takes up more of my time when I'm already struggling

with too much work ... it rarely helps [anyway] as I can't explain why I'm struggling,

otherwise I would have just done it [myself] in the first place" (Respondent #20, ibid, p99).

Furthermore, early evidence suggests that students with dyslexia under-perform in the initial stages of tackling academic assignments by lacking effective means for 'sizing up the task', and hence poorly judge its complexity (Borkowski, et.al., 1989). Although that study was concerned with the issue amongst primary-aged children with dyslexia, it spawned enough subsequent research to suggest a 'Strategy Deficit Model' (Swanson, 1990), as a framework for understanding it, the legacy (and model-development) of which became integrated into similar research amongst the community of individuals with dyslexia (e.g.: Lienemann & Reid, 2006; Bergery, et.al., 2017). But to assume that this is an inherent difficulty that is a consequence of dyslexia excludes the possibility that the way in which the task is framed may make deciphering what to do especially challenging for individuals characterized as neurodiverse thinkers. In other words, for students with dyslexia, the challenges in properly understanding how to tackle an academic challenge, may be more a function of the manner in which the task's academic context is framed as much as any research-reported deficit in meta-cognitive awareness. It is not unreasonable to suggest that any or all of these factors are likely to impact on confidence when tackling learning challenges.

 

What is especially notable is that several conclusions drawn in this thesis have implied that many of these issues may be widespread across student communities and not necessarily more prevalent amongst those with dyslexic learning differences. What does appear to be widespread in dyslexic learners, is the enduring legacy of being ‘othered’ as a result of ‘differences’ in learning contexts, especially where this extends to stigmatization, which consequently has a detrimental impact on confidence for approaching and tackling learning tasks and challenges. Hence this thesis has attempted to demonstrate that negative self-perceptions are associated with being identified as dyslexic and that these may also have an abiding effect on depressing academic confidence, which then persists throughout subsequent, situational learning circumstances - in this case, three or more years of university study.

 

All of this is despite some signs of a genuine shift towards embracing better inclusivity in teaching and learning, not least through a wider adoption of learning development initiatives. Although these are welcome and well-intentioned, they mostly seem to remain focused on designing remedial activities to upskill the academically weak, disadvantaged or disabled - an observation based my own experience as an academic guide in three university settings over the past decade and more. Indeed, branding services as 'study skills' or 'academic support', may inadvertently reinforce the wider perception that the target audience is the struggling learner. Whereas the reality, or at least the aspiration (in my experience) is to provide wider access to learning enhancement for the whole student community. Sadly, the outcome is that most of the principles enshrined in the concept of Universal Design for Learning, greeted with wide enthusiasm and eagerness as an agent for change in learning and teaching at the turn of the century, remain largely unadopted. All of which means that learning and study regimes at university still tend to be lacking in sufficient flexibility and adaptability to more equitably accommodate learning difference, a situation which remains inherently unjust. But this is a topic for future research.

ResearchPurpose

6.2  Summarizing the research outcomes

 

This research used a self-report questionnaire, completed online by university students predominantly at one UK institution, to collect data about academic confidence and dyslexia-ness. Academic confidence was assessed using the existing ABC Scale developed by Sander and colleagues in the early 2000s with later modifications, together with locally-derived variants. Dyslexia-ness was assessed using an especially-developed Dyslexia Index (Dx) Profiler which framed dyslexia through the lens of study skills using a multi-factorial approach. By collecting background data about the general demographical distribution of students in the datapool, it was established that the sample could reasonably be considered as a typical cross-section of a student community at a UK HE institution.

 

The data collected permitted two research groups to be established: one group of self-declared dyslexic students; the other, students who declared no known dyslexic learning differences. From these, three subgroups were derived using the criteria of dyslexia-ness established from the output of the Dx Profiler. These were students with known dyslexia, validated by high levels of dyslexia-ness, (the Control subgroup); students with no known dyslexia, validated by presenting low levels of dyslexia-ness, (the Base subgroup); and students with no known dyslexia but who presented high levels of dyslexia-ness, (the Test subgroup).

 

The research questions asked firstly whether university students who know about their dyslexia present significantly lower academic confidence than their non-dyslexic peers. Secondly whether students who indicated no formally identified dyslexia but who showed strong evidence of dyslexia-like learning and study profiles, present higher levels of academic confidence than their dyslexia-identified peers. From these, a further research question emerged which asked whether or not the manner in which students with dyslexia learned of their dyslexia impacted on their levels of academic confidence.

 

Data from the self-report questionnaire were analysed using a selection of statistical processes which first established levels of reliability of the two metrics for gauging the ABC and Dx of students in this datapool using the Cohen's ɑ coefficient. Although ɑ-levels were high for both metrics, it emerged that some scale item redundancy was present in both scales. Consequently, four variants of the ABC Scale and two variants of the Dx Profiler were developed and applied to the data. Hence, several permutations of ABC outcomes with Dx outcomes became available, and rather than select one as the definitive pair, outputs from all combinations were generated. This was considered a strength of the study because it permitted a matrix of outcomes to be considered in the context of the research questions and hypotheses. This course of action was vindicated by similar results emerged whichever combination of the two metrics were chosen.

In the event, by comparing mean-average data for ABC between the groups and subgroups, it was first established that non-dyslexic students present a substantially and (statistically) significantly higher level of academic confidence than their dyslexia-identified peers. It was further established from whichever combination of ABC Scale and Dx Profiler variant used, that there was a small-to-medium effect size between ABC means of strongly dyslexic students (Control subgroup) and strongly quasi-dyslexic students (Test subgroup). Although it was not possible to declare definitively these outcomes as significant (according to conventionally defined criteria) they were sufficiently marginal to suggest that further research would be warranted using a larger datapool, or to assemble data from several studies into a meta-analysis should these become available. Hence although the second null hypothesis that there is no difference in academic confidence between dyslexic and quasi-dyslexic students could not be rejected, levels of academic confidence across the spectrum was higher for students in the quasi-dyslexic subgroup.

 

Furthermore, analysis showed a moderate, ABC effect size between dyslexic students whose dyslexia had been diagnosed to them as a disability and those who were told of their dyslexia in other ways. Hence indicating to students that their dyslexia has been diagnosed, and that the condition also categorises them as disabled, may be tacitly labelling dyslexia as an illness or mental health condition and thus, may be a significantly impacting factor that contributes towards reduced levels of academic confidence. This was an expected, and unsurprising result, confirming much of the prior, anecdotal evidence upon which the stance of this project was formulated.

ResearchOutcomes

6.3  Limitations of the research

 

To set the objective of identifying quasi-dyslexic students from a cohort of students outwardly declaring no dyslexic learning differences raised unprecedented challenges. To date, no other studies have been found that attempted such an ambitious task at this level and in this field of educational research. Hence the research design necessarily comprised many elements that were previously untested. These included the design and development of a data collection tool that could be relied upon to identify quasi-dyslexic students sensitively, whilst at the same time not constituting a dyslexia screener; and also the construction and deployment of a data collection system that could be delivered to a broad range of participants, recruited into the study in ways that were unbiased, not skewed, and hence, reasonably representative of the wider community of students at a typical university in the UK. On reflection, it is acknowledged that to attempt a project of such complexity as a sole researcher could be considered as over-ambitious and possibly beyond the requirements for study at this level. But the aims of the study emerged from a desire to contribute something of value that could add to the body of research evidence arguing for a transformation in the ways that students with learning differences - whether categorized as dyslexia or not (itself later shown to be a contentious point) are enabled and empowered to make the most of studying at university.

I  Scale limitations

Thus, it is acknowledged that the most critical limitation of the study should be attributed to the design, development and deployment of the Dx Profiler as the discriminator for finding students with quasi-dyslexia. This was an innovative and possibly controversial instrument for gauging dyslexia-ness, itself a term inaugurated in this study. Although an exhaustive process of development led to confidence in the Profiler’s ability to meet the design objective of this study, it remains untested outside this datapool of students. Nevertheless, and in addition to robust, theoretical underpinnings, an attempt was made to elicit background data from dyslexia study-support professionals working with students in universities across the UK to aid the formulation of the Profiler, even if the response from them was disappointing. With the benefit of hindsight, this process may have been more successful were the purposes and critical value of the data that were being requested to have been communicated more clearly. But in the interests of 'keeping it brief' so as to encourage participation from busy, professional tutors, it is possible that the importance of this data for developing the whole project was not communicated clearly enough. Hence, one limitation of the Dx Profiler might be attributed to this element of its underpinnings being based on somewhat scant evidence despite strong theoretical reasoning. This design intention was ambitious and due to a dearth of appropriate data for formulating it, outputs may be judged as somewhat unreliable because the weightings assigned to the Dx Profiler dimensions were derived from data collected through this design development process. Given that the aggregated, dimensional, weighted values constituted the final Dyslexia Index (Dx) for each participant in the study, obtaining the best quality data possible from which to derive the weightings would enhance the precision of the final Dx value. Clearly, more data from which to have developed the dimension weightings would have added to the reliability of the Profiler.

 

But these features of the development of the Dx Profiler have been acknowledged throughout the discussion element of this project, and a sound attempt to deal with them was demonstrated in the data analysis through the use of statistical devices pitched at strengthening the validity of the Profiler. This was not least through scale reliability analysis, which, aside from outcomes suggesting that the Profiler was reliably measuring what it set out to measure, also led to an alternative, truncated edition being devised and subsequently used in the analysis. However, it is also acknowledged that an inherent research weakness of this study may have been introduced because the design and development of the Dx Profiler probably demanded a sizeable project in its own right. Hence in this current study it has been used in its nascent form, generating outcomes that are tentative but interesting and worthy of further research. These points have also been acknowledged earlier, and development work on the Profiler will be a project for a later date.

It is also suggested that the standardized, and relatively well-used ABC Scale is itself, under-developed, which could limit the generalizability of the conclusions and outcomes established from its use in this current study. It has been argued that one aspect of the scale's immaturity stems from a concern about the applicability of the standard factor structure of the scale to datasets are not close emulations of those from which the factor structure originally emerged through Sander & Sanders' work. This possibility arose at an early stage of the review of studies that have used the ABC Scale, such that some studies utilized the original 24-item scale, whereas others used the truncated, 17-item version which has a substantially different factor structure. The importance of this lies with the dimensions of the 24-item scale that were removed because it was considered that such a retraction may have been especially datapool-specific, since no wider validation was found. Consequently, both through scale reliability analysis, and dimension reduction simulations (using the Eigenvalue Monte Carlo method), two alternative factor structures for the ABC Scale emerged that were entirely based on this study's datapool in because the simulation used randomized trials of that data. Together with the two original scales, which were considered to be perfectly usable despite the limitations raised (not least due to the legacy of their use in several prior studies), this led four distinct sets of outputs to be established. Although this was considered to be a strength of the study, it could equally be argued that this multiplicity of outputs can have conflated or obfuscated the conclusions drawn about students' academic confidence.

 

This cautious approach was a response to the need for data analysis processes to be as relevant and applicable as possible, and pays more than a passing reference to earlier attention drawn (in sub-section 2.1(VII)) to an example of the reportedly disappointing effectiveness of a construct-evaluating metric developed from a closed cohort sample at a single university, when the metric was used to explore the same construct as presented in a sample taken from a different university's student community (the YAA Adult Dyslexia Scale; Hatcher & Snowling, 2002). In that case, the scale was adapted for use in an Australian university with disappointing results (Chanock et al., 2010), attributed to the limitations of the metric due to its development having been based entirely on data collected from a single source. The argument followed that this reduced its adaptability for use in outwardly similar contexts but where (as in that case), significant differences in test-subject demographics appeared sufficient to upset the results. Despite this concern, and in keeping with comments above about the Dx Profiler, the four versions of the ABC Scale used in this current study were considered to be a strength of the analysis process. This was firstly because the two locally-derived scales were exactly pertinent to the locally collected data, and hence their outputs might be considered as those most likely to reflect the true characteristics of academic confidence of the students in this datapool; and secondly because, as with the two versions of the Dx Profiler considered of equal merit, the simultaneous outputs generated from the same variables could be collectively compared. Hence, both local ABC outputs have also been included in the results and analysed where apposite. There has been neither the time nor scope in this current study to explore the differences and similarities that emerged from ABC Scales' differences in detail - which is considered as a further limitation. Early indications suggest there may be merit in reviewing and more deeply analysing the data, which remains another possible topic for future study.

II  Data collection, sampling and datapool limitations; measurement issues;

Due to the researcher's geographical location, this was a distance-learning study conducted remotely from the data source - namely students attending the same, home university. By its very nature, this advocated the design of a data collection process that could also be conducted remotely. To have adopted an alternative method, such as face-to-face structured interviews, or personally canvassing for participation in a paper-based or electronically-derived questionnaire for example, would have been unworkable. But in different circumstances, either of these alternatives may have been equally productive in terms of the breadth, quality and detail of data that could have been collected. In the event, a great deal of thought, preparation, and prior technical expertise was invested in designing and developing a self-report, electronically-deliverable questionnaire that was technically fault-free, attractive to view, easy to navigate, simple to understand, complete, and submit, and not over-burdensome in either time required to complete it, nor the complexity or wordiness of its constituent components. Alternative data collection processes were considered, but from the outset this study was designed to be a primary research project where the more data that could be collected the better. Thus adopting a case-study approach for example, where the same research questions might have been meaningfully explored, was dismissed at an early stage because although data collected using this approach could have been high-quality, it would have emerged from sources that may not have been a representative cross-section of students at university. It was difficult to imagine how the core, Test subgroup of quasi-dyslexic students may have been established using this approach. Hence, generalizable outcomes were considered less likely to emerge through this methodology. However, it is acknowledged that if such a research design could have been formulated, it may have generated equivalent outcomes if less wide-ranging in value, that is, with a alternative, more individually-focused emphasis.

Hence, collecting data through a self-report questionnaire, electronically delivered and submitted was considered the most viable option. Considerable credence was given to pitfalls and limitations attributed to collecting attitudinal data in this way (reported earlier, sub-section 3.3) and attention was paid to accounting for, and designing these out wherever possible. However, it is acknowledged that such a data collection process brings its own limitations. These include the extent to which participants respond to questions honestly, on their own (without any help, or prompting), and in full understanding of the content, structure and purpose of the enquiry. These factors are beyond the control of the researcher, and hence variability in data quality and response veracity represents a source of potential limitation that may impact on the generalizability of the conclusions. Another limiting factor is that overall, the target audience was unknown. However, for the group of dyslexic students at least, this was partially under the control of the researcher. These participants were assumed to be definitely identified as dyslexic by virtue of them being targeted for recruitment through the university's Dyslexia Service e-mail distribution list. But for an individual in receipt of the e-mail invitation to join the study, choosing to participate was voluntary. Hence, it was not possible to devise and access a non-probability, purposive sample that would have been logically assumed to have been representative of the background population of all students with dyslexia attending the home university. In any case, such a process was precluded by the Service who, certainly at the early stages of scoping the data collection process, were reluctant to be involved in the study at all, citing potential breaches of confidentiality as the reason. This was despite assurances (from the study's supervisory team) that data collected through the questionnaire were completely, irrevocably and unconditionally anonymised, with no possible route to trace responses back to an identifiable individual.

 

Thus, it was not possible to determine the extent to which those who were recruited to the study through this route represented a reasonably random cross-section of students with dyslexia at the university. Hence it is acknowledged that the outcomes of the analysis could be skewed as a consequence. For example it was known that the greater proportion of respondents who were recruited through this route were female, outnumbering males by a factor of three to one. But without privileged access to the gender distribution of students registered with the university's Dyslexia Service (a request for which was submitted but no response received), it was impossible to determine how representative this ratio was, and hence, whether males were disproportionately under-represented. For students recruited into the non-dyslexic group it was at least possible to determine that the distribution of participants by gender was approximately representative of the university student population nationally, as these data were available from HESA for comparison.

The sample size was considered as a moderately large (n=166) for a research study of this type, as stated in the literature review. When the datapool was sifted into subgroups, sample sizes reduced, although with n=98, and n=68 non-dyslexic, and dyslexic students in each subgroup respectively, these were still considered to be sufficiently large for the outcomes of the data analysis to be meaningful and worthy of interpretation. However, given that one of the principal aims of the study was to identify quasi-dyslexic students from the non-dyslexic group, it was noted from the outset that the size of the that subgroup was likely to be quite small. Hence conclusions drawn would need to acknowledge that small samples are likely to provide evidence for tentative outcomes only. In the event, sifting quasi-dyslexic students from their parent group generated a Test subgroup of 18, or 19 participants, according to which version of the Dx Profiler was applied as the discriminator. Nevertheless, this subgroup represented a substantial proportion of its parent group (18.4%), which, were quasi-dyslexia considered as likely unidentified dyslexia, may suggest the proportion of unknown dyslexia could be as high as nearly one in five supposedly non-dyslexic students at university. However, drawing such a conclusion from this data was considered speculative, not least because the sample is small and the validity of the discriminator is untested outside this datapool.

Limitations

6.4  Directions for future research

 

It is believed that this study is the first to explore the possible relationship between academic confidence and dyslexia amongst a community of university students.

 

This was a unique investigation. Further work is required within the domain of higher education to validate the findings of this current study or otherwise provide evidence to contradict them. Within this, the idea of 'dyslexia-ness' prompts a wider discussion. Proposed in this study as a continuum variable to gauge the prevalence of characteristics typically associated with dyslexia this is in contrast to the more conventional, categorical descriptors of the severity of dyslexia, typically required as a means to gain financial support at university through the Disabled Students' Allowance. It has been argued that the continuum approach provides a more nuanced and informed view of the syndrome as a learning difference. It is suggested that taking this more inclusive viewpoint could contribute to de-stigmatizing dyslexia, notably by distancing the syndrome from disability, and thus likely reduce feelings of 'otherness', commonly reported amongst students with dyslexia. In this study dyslexia-ness has been used to quantify the prevalence and magnitude-of-influence of a variety of the learning and study characteristics frequently impacted by dyslexic learning differences - but not necessarily absent from apparently non-dyslexic individuals. This perspective is particularly apposite when the syndrome is taken as a multifactorial, information processing difference.

In the current study, the focus of the investigation has been to explore the legitimacy of relating dyslexia-ness to academic confidence Further work needs to be conducted to consider whether such an interrelationship is meaningful in higher education contexts, and whether the variables taken together and presented in a relatable way could be valid and useful baselines across learning development enterprises more widely available to all university students. This inter-relationship could be developed into an individualized, profiling format, and preliminary work to explore the visualization of such profiles has commenced using the data collected in this study, with findings to date reported in the Addenda section of this thesis (in progress). The focus of this work will be to determine whether such profiling can have a useful and productive impact on guiding students at university to understand more about their own learning strategies, strengths, and weaknesses. The research design would be formulated to explore how such meta-knowledge might be channelled into formal learning development processes aimed at shepherding students towards study mechanisms that might clear their learning blockages whilst simultaneously enhance the effectiveness and efficacy of their study strategies. It is not unreasonable to assume that the outcome of such initiatives would be a more effective and productive academic journey through time spent at university, the outcomes of which would then be true and proper indicators of a student's academic competency and capabilities.

Equally, further work needs to be undertaken to develop both of the metrics used in this current study. The ABC Scale, although already established, could be updated to reflect the shifts in teaching and learning regimes at university, for example. Since the scale was originally developed in the early 2000s, learning systems in HE have progressed, reflecting greater use of curriculum delivery through electronic and social media applications with a reduction in some large-lecture instruction. Additionally, very recent developments in curriculum delivery have witnessed a wider adoption of blended learning models with an increasing proportion of student teaching being presented online in many institutions in the UK. So the ABC Scale could be adapted to reflect these changes whilst at the same time retaining its underpinning ethos for gauging the effectiveness of students' learning strategies and study behaviours through the lens of academic confidence. Recall that academic confidence has been cited as a significant factor in the self-regulation of learning.

 

The Dyslexia Index Profiler was developed especially for this project and although it has served the purpose for which it was designed, it remains untested more widely. So in the first instance, it is recommended that more data should be amassed from university dyslexia support professionals to enable the dimensions that comprise the Profiler to reflect more accurately the prevalence of the characteristics and attributes that they are gauging. Secondly, a wider deployment of the Profiler across a greater range of students at university would enable a better picture to be established of the extent to which all students can be located on the Dyslexianess Continuum. Hence, a deeper investigation of the factor structure of the Dx Profiler could be undertaken, as the data available in this study were only sufficient to hint that a factor structure may be determinable. As a result of refinements of the scale, it may be possible to suggest how the understanding of dyslexia in adult learners might be reframed more positively, especially if this can lead to a wider debate about how appropriate or useful it is to formally identify the syndrome in university contexts - no doubt a contentious point. Given that current convention leans towards retaining a process of identification, a deeper exploration of the impact of 'diagnosing' the syndrome is recommended, especially where this combined with dyslexia being defined as a disability and disclosed as such to the individual concerned. Perhaps more evidence to support the conclusions of this current study in this respect might encourage a more widespread shift towards describing dyslexia more neutrally to those identified with it.

Lastly, there remains a considerable quantity of data collected in this study that warrant deeper analysis. For example, at present, assessments of the interrelationships between dyslexia-ness and academic confidence have been mostly confined to the complete scales although the factor structure of the ABC Scale has been explored and accommodated into the analysis. But outcomes for ABC Scale item dimensions are available, which could lead to a more detailed exploration of the differences in ABC that have been revealed between non-dyslexic and dyslexic students. Knowing more about this might provide a deeper understanding about how identified dyslexia impacts on learners' confidence when approaching specific components of learning within the wider regimes at university.

Directions

6.5  Concluding remarks

 

What do the outcomes of this research say about the academic confidence of students at university? What has emerged about the nature of dyslexia in students at university, and how has this contributed to what is already known about how this substantial minority of learners function and engage with university study? Specifically, what has been revealed about the inter-relationships between these two variables? And has enough been established to speculate, in a reasonably informed way, about how university teaching and learning could be developed in the light of evidence presented in this current study?

 

A primary aim of this project was to reveal differences in academic confidence between non-dyslexic and dyslexic students; an additional aim was to explore the effects that attributing the label of dyslexia to a particular set of learning and study profiles might have on academic confidence. This may be of critical importance in the field of learning design in higher education contexts because by establishing substantial, even significant differences, it may be possible to infer that a reduced likelihood of gaining strong academic outcomes may be at least partially attributable to lower levels of academic confidence, which, as a sub-construct of academic self-efficacy, has been previously reported as a potential marker for academic performance (Honicke & Broadbent, 2016). Hence it would be reasonable to suggest that minimizing impacts that can be shown to depress academic confidence - of which identifying dyslexia maybe one - are likely to have a positive affect on academic achievement.

 

In short, when it comes to guiding learners towards a good degree at university, this project has established that amongst the community of learners at one university at least, and based on one reasonably sized datapool, there may be in inverse relationship between levels of dyslexia-ness and levels of academic confidence. The study has also questioned whether it is better to label an individual as ‘dyslexic’ or not, and has shown that the answer to this may not be as straightforward as previously imagined. By locating all participants in this study on the Dyslexianess Continuum and attempting to identify a discrete subgroup of individuals presenting quasi-dyslexia-ness, it has been shown that in some cases, learning differences that might be attributable to dyslexia may be best left unidentified, notably if academic confidence is the gauge. It logically follows that for these individuals, and possibly dyslexic students generally, it may be more appropriate (in relation to learning outcomes at least) for them to remain unaware of their ‘learning difference’ and that these learners should be encouraged to battle on as best they can within the literacy-based system of curriculum delivery in which they are studying. This, despite the learning environent being unsuited to their learning characteristics, strengths and preferences. To take this approach is undoubtedly controversial, but evidence presented in this study suggests that undertaking a formal dyslexia assessment, possibly leading to a 'dyslexia diagnosis' may outweigh the apparent benefits of remaining ignorant of the fact. It logically follows that were this course of action to become the norm, the conventional, and undoubtedly well-intentioned notion of ‘reasonable adjustments’ would become redundant and obselete. Such students with dyslexia would no longer be marked as different from their peers, not least because their learning difference, however it may be defined, would remain unidentified. Notably, such students would be less likely to be 'othered', and sometimes rejected by their peers in co-operative learning initiatives on the basis of a learning difference. Evidence for this has been presented throughout this thesis, from prior literature, from the previous Masters level small-scale study, and of course, from this current study.

 

To identify or not is further compounded in that dyslexia remains difficult to define because it can comprise a variety of arguably identifiable characteristics which can occur together in multiple combinations. These may not have discrete impacts on learning, and in some cases may be comorbid with other situational conditions or personal circumstances, the impacts of which may be equally challenging to ameliorate or even to begin to define. It has also been shown that some of these profiles of dimensions are observable in non-dyslexic students too. In academically capable individuals, the impacts of the more conventionally considered characteristics of dyslexia associated with weak literacy skills can have been significantly ameliorated, either through strategically modifying intrinsic approaches to learning, consciously or unconsciously, or through use of external support resources such as digital and assistive technologies. The outcome is that many of the earlier issues that a dyslexic individual might have faced in their learning history may have been significantly diminished. This has been readily demonstrated when dyslexia is considered as a multifactorial learning difference, whereby individuals can present significant levels of dyslexia-ness in some factors but not in others. Whilst dyslexia continues to be difficult to define, the value to the individual of identifying it, assessing it and somehow quantifying its severity or magnitude of influence, seems dubious.

 

This leads to an acknowledgement that dyslexia at university might be best considered as an information processing difference rather than predominantly defined a literacy-skills deficiency. But the literacy demands of academic study continue to present disadvantageous conditions for many students with information processing differences, because curricula are still broadly delivered and assessed in literacy-based formats. A more appropriate repackaging of dimensions of dyslexia-ness in a contemporary university-learning context may be to consider these characteristics more broadly as academic learning management dimensions, not least because many of them are  observable across the wide diversity of university student communities. By characterizing any student’s blend of dimensions through a profile approach, based on a continuum interpretation of dyslexia-ness and academic confidence for example, a better understanding of academic strengths and weaknesses might be established. Subsequently, this could be the agent for learning development strategies to be designed and individually-tailored that would capitalize on strengths and ameliorate weaknesses, and hence enhance the effectiveness of learning, enable students to gain a working understanding of their own meta-learning, and to reflect on how this knowledge about how they learn best can be developed into learning actions. On this basis, comprehensive, personalized learning plans could be developed through the lens of dyslexia-ness and academic confidence, an approach which to date, it is believed remains unconsidered. Such learning plans would emerge as useful not just for students with dyslexia (were it deemed still necessary to formally identify them) but for anyone studying at university. Since academic confidence is “a mediating variable that acts between individuals' inherent abilities, their learning styles and opportunities afforded by the academic environment of higher education” (Sander & Sanders, 2003, p4), gaining a greater understanding of how it impacts on academic outcomes would be a conduit for enhancing these outcomes and creating a more fulfilling and less stressful learning experience. Ultimately this could promote better academic achievements that are more likely to accurately represent individuals' abilities and capabilities. Granted, this may challenge the scope of strategic planning for the future of tertiary-level, high-quality learning by being regarded as too radical and expensive to implement, and/or inhibited by organizational and systemic factors that are resistant to change (Simons et al., 2007).

In short, as universities have opened their doors to a broader spectrum of students through widening participation and alternative access schemes (which have also seen a substantial rise in numbers of students with learning differences choosing to enter HE), it is reasonable to suppose that many of these new faces, in addition to many of the more traditionally-seen ones, would benefit academically were there a better institutional-level understanding of the impact that individual differences can have on educational engagement and ownership of learning (Conley & French, 2014). Adopting the principles of UDL would meet many of these objectives by ensuring a more accessible, flexible and adaptable learning provision at university that would enable not only students with dyslexia but all students to engage more equitably with learning, using the academic and functional capabilities that they bring to their institutions, unhindered by burdens of judgemental 'difference-identification', or any other potentially marginalizing factor.

ConcludingRemarks
-----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
bottom of page